THE COURSE PROJECT IS DUE WEDNESDAY MAY 2 BY NOON
ABSOLUTE FINAL DUE DATE. Slightly early papers ARE accepted.
turnitin is up on our Canvas site. It's at the top of the Assignments folder: Milestone 4
If you have any problems with turnitin, please let me know ASAP!

PRESENTATIONS ARE SUNDAY APRIL 15 and 22 at 2 AND WEDNESDAY APRIL 18 and 25 12-2. ALL ARE WELCOME!


OVERVIEW
READINGS
EDP 5285 GROUP PROCESSES

GUIDE 1:  INTRODUCTION
GUIDE 2: METHODS FOR STUDYING GROUPS
GUIDE 3: GROUP STRUCTURE
GUIDE 4: ASPECTS OF GROUP STRUCTURE II
GUIDE 5: ATTRACTION TO GROUPS
GUIDE 6: COHESIVENESS II
GUIDE 7: INFLUENCE PROCESSES
GUIDE 8: PERFORMANCE & DECISION-MAKING
GUIDE 9: LEADERSHIP
GUIDE 10: GROUP COOPERATION & CONFLICT


COURSE PROJECT
PRESENTATION

EDP5285-01  SPRING 2018
PROFESSOR SUSAN CAROL LOSH


YES! TIME FLIES!
THE FINAL DUE DATE FOR THE FINAL DRAFT OF THE COURSE PROJECT IS 
WEDNESDAY MAY 2 NOON.
THROUGH THE TURNITIN LINK IN OUR CANVAS ASSIGNMENTS: Milestone 4 SITE

 
GROUP PROCESSES

 
GUIDE TO THE MATERIAL: TEN
ACROSS, WITHIN: GROUP COOPERATION AND CONFLICT

 

KEY TAKEAWAYS:
  • Conflict ALSO occurs within groups as well as across groups
    • Internal factions may even split or destroy a group
  • Cross group conflict may occur whether groups anticipate future interaction, or no interaction
    • When no future interaction anticipated, less of a feeling of accountability
    • Intergroup conflict then can escalate
  • It is much easier to create conflict than to solve cross group conflict
    • Traditional hostilities, scarce resources, strong social identities, some world views (e.g., "life is a jungle fight")
  • Creating cooperation: what DOESN'T work:
    • Simple contact
    • Contact under enjoyable circumstances (e.g., a movie or banquet)
    • Positive propaganda
  • Creating cooperation: what MIGHT work:
    • Equal status contact
    • Superordinate goals
    • "Jigsaw" problems
    • A problem-solving approach
    • GIVE IT TIME!
  • Cognitive modifiers
    • Stereotypes in particular
    • Resistant to change--a lens to view the world!
    • Ideologically justify cross group conflict and poor treatment of outgroup
    • Groupthink may contribute
    • Social identity may contribute
  • Conflict CAN have positive outcomes for all groups
 

TOPICS
WITHIN GROUPS, ACROSS GROUPS
CREATING CONFLICT
CREATING COOPERATION
COGNITIVE MODIFIERS
GROUPTHINK REVISITED



Muzafer Sherif's classic "Robber's Cave" experiment remains one of the most famous studies in the group dynamics literature. "Well-adjusted" White, 11 year old American boys in the mid-1950s had a camp experience that those who are still around probably remember to this day. They were brought to camp in two groups and each group was initially separated from the other. Over several days, each group became aware of the other. In a matter of days, generously assisted by the research team, cross-group rivalries developed, culminating in a "tournament." Curiosity gave way to the development of social identity and in-group loyalty (group names and symbols) and considerable outgroup animosity. The research team then faced the task of bringing the two groups peacefully together, which they eventually accomplished through a series of engineered problems that required cross-group cooperation to solve.

To this day, I wonder how successful the research team truly was in creating cooperation--or did they fudge a little about how much they succeeded? After all, had children gone home prior to the series of engineered disasters designed to bring the groups together, what would they have told their parents about the camp experience? Would the researchers have been sued, despite the signed consent forms? This series of studies was conducted about 20 years prior to the establishment of institutional review boards (IRBs or Human Subjects Committees) on campus. Would they have been approved? What kind of arguments would you have presented as the Principal Investigator to convince the FSU IRB to approve this research?
 


WITHIN GROUPS, ACROSS GROUPS
WHAT KINDS OF DIFFERENCES?

Typically we think of conflict as occuring across groups and cooperation as occuring within groups. Yet factions and exploitation can also occur within groups even with as few as three members, making cooperation among members difficult. At its worse, the group may become totally dysfunctional and even disintegrate. While we may at first consider the arguments and conflicts of interest that occur within informal friendship groups, here are some formal organizational group examples:

On the flip side, groups may have a history of intermittent or regular intergroup cooperation. The National Council of Churches encouraged working relationships among several liberal to moderate interpretive Christian denominations. Colleges and universities agree to have regular schedules of sports competition with each other in leagues. Neighborhoods may cooperate in "fighting City Hall" on urban development.
 
ANTICIPATED VERSUS UNANTICIPATED CONTACT 

While continued competitive interactions may create conflict, many consequences probably depend on whether these competitions are within or across groups, and whether these competitions are regularized in some way. Internal competition may be detrimental to group cohesion and lead to members resenting one another. In classrooms, we see this in the use of strict "curves" for grading. " Win-lose" evaluations (zero-sum matrices) or rating systems in employment and other group endeavors can sometimes even lead to the sabotage of others' efforts or "colleagues" denigrating the efforts of their peers.

Although one might suppose that internal conflict might be greater under conditions of scarce resources (see below), some recent research has found it is the rankings and relative proportions that matter to many members (about 1/4)--even if the total payoff is less for individual group members.

On the other hand, what about teams which play each other on a regular basis, where competition is expected, or "traditional rivalries" such as "brain bowls?" (Or the Seminoles versus the Gators.) Corporations, too, are regularly ranked by several agencies. Perhaps surprisingly, competition can be "good-natured" as well as "cutthroat."

The Johnsons suggest that a "go for the win" strategy or distributed negotiation is most pronounced when parties do not regularly interact and do not anticipate future interaction. Under these circumstances groups do not see mutual interests and may believe that their own negative actions will not have consequences. On the other hand, integrative negotiation, or problem solving attempts to maximize joint benefits should be more common when regular interaction is expected. For example, football teams from adjacent high schools, whose members interact in several other contexts, may still want to win but may also be less likely to engage in truly "dirty" ploys. Supportive evidence has been reported in summaries by sociologist Joe Feagin. In another example, the FSU faculty union (United Faculty of Florida-FSU) uses "interest based bargaining" or shared interests in collective bargaining with the FSU administration.
 

CREATING CONFLICT

For whatever reasons, it appears much easier to create conflict among groups than to create cooperation. Here are some factors that increase animosity across groups:

Are some people particularly prone to create or aggrevate group conflict? Lawrence Wrightman's philosphies of human nature refer to individuals who see the world as an untrustworthy place. Such individuals tend to go into situations with a "win-lose" perspective rather than a focus on shared problem solving. Of course, unwittingly, these "jungle fighters" can create the very competitive and abrasive situations that they claim to find so abhorrent. Others react to these dominance attempts and what is easily perceived as outright GREED with escalated competition and rancor--and the battle is on. Unfortunately, Prisoner's Dilemma Game studies indicate that it is easier to lose trust and create competitive situations than it is to create cooperative relationships. Once lost, trust is not easily regained.
 
CREATING COOPERATION

 
WHAT DOESN'T WORK

Unfortunately, it is tougher to create cooperation than conflict. If one approach does not work, the advice is: try, try again with something else. Keep in mind that adversarial parties in conflict often don't want to meet, don't want to cooperate. In an atmosphere of suspicion, each side fears that any attempts at reconciliation will be seen as weakness or concessions, leading the "advance party" worse off than before.

WHAT CAN HELP

Try some of these suggestions and remember to give any of them some time. It's easier to instill suspicion than trust (repeat!).


COGNITIVE MODIFIERS

 
IN-GROUP AND OUT-GROUP COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Several cognitive processes can foster cross-group conflict.

These processes can unwittingly support cross-group conflict. But probably the most studied cognitive phenomenon with respect to intergroup relations is stereotyping. Stereotypes are a special type of cognitive prototype applied to large social groups.
 
HOW STEREOTYPES WORK

When we stereotype, we categorize. We take the people we encounter and place them in a "cognitive bin" with information about other people whom we believe are similar in key respects (such as gender, ethnicity, sexual orientation, occupation or age). This is all well and good--people categorize all kinds of objects as a form of cognitive "economy" (do you examine every head of lettuce at the store? or each tube of Colgate toothpaste?). However, stereotypes differ in key sinister ways from other forms of categorization:

THE ROLE OF COGNITION: CAUSE OR CONSEQUENCE?

Psychology typically locates the genus of action in the individual. Thus, psychologists will treat stereotypes and other ideological factors held by individuals as causal to outgroup hostility and scapegoating. Social psychologists recognize that some causes of our behavior are internal but that others emanate from the environment. Further, social psychology has shown that if people change their behavior (perhaps due to outside forces) and feel committed to that behavior, attitudes will often follow suit to become consistent with the behavior.

What does all this mean for the rhetoric of outgroup hostilities? Some conflictual actions toward outgroups no doubt occur because we hold pre-existing negative attitudes toward a particular group. However, it is also possible that because we treat members of a group poorly, we develop hostile attitudes toward them. To admit that we engaged in aggression toward members of another group (including cheating at games, stealing possessions, or even murder) because we were jealous, coveted their resources, felt bad because we had failed at something, or wanted an advantage toward some type of prize could induce considerable shame, guilt and anxiety. To ameliorate these nasty feelings, we convince ourselves that members of the other group "deserve it" because of their derogatory characteristics. Thus, we feel better about ourselves and our actions. We may even glorify ill behavior toward outgroups, convincing ourselves that "society is better off."
 


A RETURN TO GROUPTHINK

Once again we return to the combination of strong interpersonal cohesion and highly directive leadership that can produce "Groupthink." Several elements that Irving Janis idenitified in the syndrome were cognitive characteristics: heightened in-group identification; "we-they" thinking; a tendency to denigrate other groups and to see them as inferior to one's own. One's own group was seen as relatively invulnerable.

This perspective is also endorsed by many social identity theorists.

One guilty party that contributes so much of the research in this section is a strong sense of group identity. Is cross-group antagonism inevitable? Some social and behavior scientists think that this is so, because competition constantly occurs for valued and scarce goods. On the other hand, insights from game theory and negotiating suggest cautious optimism. By changing the definition of the situation, we may be able to maintain cohesive groups yet minimize outgroup hostility.
 
A SIDE BAR FROM GAME THEORY

Game theorists (e.g., The Prisoner's Dilemma) work with a variety of "payoff matrices" to study how groups and individuals interact. In "zero-sum" games, what one person or group wins, the other loses. This is true in most team sports, wherein only one team can win the game or the tournament. However, zero-sum is not necessarily the case in individual sports such as marathon running, where players are relatively satisfied if they set a "personal best."

In non-zero-sum games, it is possible for all parties to win something, although the payoffs may vary to the different groups. Although the naive outsiders often believe that labor and management wage constant war, in fact unions provide a structured and ritualized bargaining situation for both union and administration to "come to the table." Under these circumstances, agreements may be hammered out so that both parties believe they have gained. Superordinate goals tend to create non-zero-sum games.

Of course, so much depends on the relative resources of all parties to the interaction. If a group holds few bargaining chips, there is little incentive for the more powerful group to bargain. The less powerful group may be able to appeal to altruism or to guilt over prior treatment. Less powerful groups may be able to form coalitions  (for example, the "Tea Party" with the economically conservative among Republicans, and minorities and working class of diverse ethnic backgrounds among Democrats) thus making them more equal to groups holding more resources.

Research from some bargaining studies suggests that for as many as 25% of us, what counts is "doing better" than others--even if everyone is worse off. It's still unclear what predicts individuals who take this kind of unproductive stance.
 
IS CONFLICT ALL BAD?

Finally, it is important to remember that, although conflict is typically treated as destructive and to be avoided, it can be positive for groups. One does not necessary have to take the Johnsons' position that conflict prevents life from getting boring! (Sometimes boring has a lot of appeal.) Whether across or between groups, conflict can:




 


OVERVIEW

READINGS

This page was built with Netscape Composer.
Susan Carol Losh April 14 2018