in instruction and elsewhere INSTRUCTOR: DR. SUSAN CAROL LOSH SPRING 2018 |
Department of Educational
Psychology and Learning Systems
Florida State University
GROUP PROCESSES |
INTRODUCTION |
This site is the first of 10 course guides. They cover what we would often consider a "class lecture" preserved and presented for you on a series of Canvas web sites available 24/7 and we will be able as a class to "forge forward." In any case where I may disagreed with your readings, or may use different terminology, that will also be in these guides..Students are responsible for the information in each course guide.
Each course Guide has an associated short introductory video--just click on the thumbnail in the Guide to load it.
Check our Canvas site too for other links,
presentations from prior years, and your presentations.
|
|
|
|
NOTE: Italicized and colored phrases are part of the professional lexicon you should know!
|
Groups are one of the most "human" things about us. We are social animals. Indeed, developmental research from the 1940s indicated that orphans who were deprived of most close human social contact had a much higher death rate than those where the infants were systematically picked up and cuddled. Even when we are alone, even in the midst of our most private thoughts, the impact of groups is present:
For example:
As a union representative, I find that I am much more assertive than I am in my "other lives." And I have been a positive dragon when it came to soliciting funds for Sheltie Rescue!
In his analysis of rape statistics over 25 years ago Wolfgang Kohler discovered that rape is frequently, if not predominantly, a "social crime," i.e., at least two men in his research jointly initiate an assault.
Jane Hood tackled the "myth of the lone scholar." Perhaps you thought that scholars, including scientists, typically read, think, analyze, and write alone; Hood among other scholars finds scholarship to be a highly social and team enterprise.
The United States has a strong ideology of individual determinism, so we don't always recognize--or like to admit--how important groups are to us and for us. Many other cultures emphasize the influence of groups on the individual. An individual's achievement may be for her or his family, not a way of garnering individual mobility. Similarly, disgusting or demeaning acts on an individual's part may disgrace his or her family.
Not all collections
of individuals are groups. The
phenomena of cohorts, aggregates and groups are often treated interchangably.
This is a mistake because groups have properties the other two entities
do not. Further, confusing, say, a targeted cohort with a crowd with a
group only obfuscates the differences among these concepts.
|
Social Facilitation has become a "catchall" phrase that refers to the effects of having other people** in one's immediate physical environment. The work of Robert Zajonc (pronounced "Zy-ontz"), among others, indicates that the mere presence of other people is physically arousing. Borrowing from several learning theories (especially that of the late behaviorist Clark Hull), many behavioral scientists assert that moderate amounts of physical anxiety enhance performance. Many studies have found that moderate amounts of anxiety increase the drive or motivational component of the learning/performance equation, thus enhancing the performance of the best-learned response. Too little arousal, and the organism is not motivated, too much and it can be "flooded" by anxiety.
**The lowly cockroach is influenced by the presence of other 'roaches and also performs better when an audience of its fellows is underneath the 'roach maze. See below.
Thus, we have studied:
audience effects, where the actor performs and the others are observers. When you present before a class, you experience audience effects.
There are also co-action effects. Co-actors individually perform the same actions at the same time, typically in the same place. Think: group of students in a room writing an exam.
Both types of situations now would be defined as aggregates (see below) and use processes of social facilitation.
In both audience effects and co-action, there is no real division of labor, no coordinated cooperation, in short, neither really resembles what we usually think of as a group. Sometimes co-actors are also in aggregates, however (see below).
The arousal effects
of having others present may interfere with learning although they
can benefit performance.
For example, as a consequence of his series of studies, Zajonc advised
students to study alone (rather than in the library or in a study hall)
but to take an exam in a group situation, where the arousal effects of
co-actors will facilitate the performance of well-learned responses.
|
We can apply some of this material to anger management situations such as domestic violence. We know that domestic violence runs in families, and that abused children more often become abusers as adults. This phenomenon is most often interpreted as a case of modeling or imitation learning. However, modeling emphasizes reward issues in the model and the imitator and in this case, issues in reward are clearly questionable or at best ambiguous. Most adults who grew up in homes where spousal violence (and often child abuse) occurred rarely approve, and it is unclear exactly what is rewarding about such a situation to anyone, including perpetrators. It is not even clear if such a family of orientation situation contributes to disinhibition in family situations later (although it will influence expectations about family behavior). But we can take our application of social facilitation a step further:
Much family behavior is typically private, taking place "behind closed doors." So one of our widest repertoires of family behavior and our best-learned responses emerges from what we each observed in our own families. Because of privacy, we know little about other families' intimate lives. Horribly, another set of learned family behaviors comes from television. Mass media tend to present glossy, idealized, over dramatized, ludicrously unbelievable ("The Beverly Hillbillies" "My Name is Earl"), or dysfunctional versions of family life. Considering that some TV families engage in continual mutual insults, that The Three Stooges is still present in reruns, that children treat adults like cretins, and the worst excesses against family members appear to be excusable with ("Raymond") "sorry, Babe," the American public is treated to a terrible series of scripts for familial interaction.When disagreements and conflicts erupt in families, emotions run high and everyone is in a state of physiological arousal. That is exactly the time when well-learned responses are most often performed. If those responses are violent, their probability of occurrence is enhanced. Thus, I suspect that social facilitation adds a necessary component for how family violence is transmitted across generations to modeling approaches. While modeling may explain how the behavior is acquired, social facilitation can explain why it is performed at the time when it is.
One solution is to expand work on anger management currently introduced into schools as early as elementary school. Children learn to identify angry feelings and how to express or handle them in more positive ways. I would suggest going a step or two further (and some teachers do), introducing role-playing and rehearsal. Simply reading about alternative behaviors is probably insufficient because reading by itself keeps such behaviors 'way down in the response hierarchy. Role-playing and rehearsal in small groups can provide the practice to raise more peacable behaviors in the response repertoire when social facilitation effects make them more likely to be performed.
|
It doesn't have to be other people. My favorite example of Robert Zajonc's work on social facilitation involves audience effects with our old local friends: cockroaches. I was privileged to observe first-hand trials in his studies with Palmetto Bugs. You will be interested to know that the researchers had great difficulties keeping our Palmetto Bugs alive in chilly Michigan. Zajonc and his students trained cockroaches to "run" toward a light in a clear lucite t-maze. Those of us who have shared a residence with 'roaches can testify that they normally AVOID light. An "audience box" was located beneath the t-maze where the 'roach marathoners could see, and presumably smell, their colleagues. 'Roaches ran their t-mazes faster and more accurately with an audience of their fellow 'roaches than when no audience was present.And then, there are rats (well, lab rats anyway) who have shown themselves in 2017 research to be empathetic, enabling a captive rat to escape and even sharing chocolate chips with the former captive!
|
Members of a cohort share at least one common characteristic or experience. Demographers usually reserve the term for people who have a common experience at a singular point in time, such as a birth cohort ("generation") or students who entered Florida State University as freshmen in the Fall of 2017. But other examples include the "soccer mom" vote, i.e., collections of individuals with a common characteristic. Members of a cohort may never meet one another, may never even gather in the same place, and generally do not think of themselves as "group members." In fact, members of a cohort may be quite diverse (are all soccer moms the same?) because other characteristics or experiences cross-cut what they have in common. However, cohort members may self identify with their cohort ("I'm a Generation Xer" or "I'm the soccer mom"). It is likely that our memories of Septemer 11, 2001 will in part set those of us who watched them aside from future generations, just as memories of the Great Depression and Pearl Harbor in part defined many people who are now senior citizens. This self-identification with a cohort can influence behavior through collective identification. We'll examine Social Identity Theory in later weeks. Politicians and market researchers are well aware of cohorts, and use them to target segments of the population for advertising and other persuasion campaigns (e.g., the "White working class male" in the recent US Presidential election.) Cohort experiences may also make it easier for people to get to know each other when they first meet, by providing common experiences.
Aggregates are collections of individuals who are physically proximate at the same time. These could be an aggregate in the Stone Building elevator or a crowd at an FSU football game. Examples are:
Audience
effects
Co-action
effects
Crowd
effects
Fads
Historical studies of riots and revolutions show that crowd behavior can be the spark that topples governments or creates a night of terror. There is at least the suggestion that rape, which can be a group perpetrated event, may be an example of crowd behavior. Studies of these events often reference the arousing effects of others, the apparent normlessness of crowds, and the "confrontational nature" of the events (i.e., spontaneous, unplanned, and immediate).
If the right circumstances are in place, a crowd or a cohort may become a group. If you are with a group of strangers and the FSU Stone Building elevator begins to play scarey games, you will probably quickly become a group . Members of a crowd can coalesce into revolutionaries. The September 11 attacks joined many New Yorkers, and symbols of American patriotism (flags, bumper stickers, lapel pins) spontaneously and quickly became common, at least for a while.
But it takes
more than just a bunch of people together in the same place to be a group.
As
you can see, aggregates and cohorts do influence behavior. They are phenomena
clearly worthy of study--but they typically aren't groups, although they
are sometimes confused with groups. We should separate these concepts carefully
so that we don't misapply them.
Group or aggregate?
We need more information here! (And no clones need apply.)
|
Almost
certainly a group (two groups actually). Why?
|
Here are my keys to defining a group. They are parallel although not identical to components that both your texts mention. Many psychologists, such as Don Forsyth, stress interaction (especially face-to-face interaction) far more than I do, partially because they may be thinking of small groups:
Without other-definition, we may perceive the person as a fraud and expel them. At the least, we may create distance mechanisms (such as ignoring the person, "rewriting history" to exclude them from the group, or literally "cutting them out of the picture.) Someone who is "in, but not of" the group is not seen as entitled to group privileges or rewards.
Interdependent
goals require coordination among the membership to
achieve them.
They require
the coordinated efforts of at least two people working together.
Interdependent
goals imply a division of labor so that each member has a unique and specific
task, i.e., a specialized task.
It is the interdependence and interlocking roles that either the group creates (in more informal groups) or that form part of a pre-existing structure (in formal groups) that meld people together into a unity.
Members of a geographical
neighborhood may face a wrecker's ball through "urban renewal" simply because
they live within a few square blocks of one another, although this aggregate
experience, too, may be an impetus to later group formation.
Other defining
characteristics include:
I define cohesiveness as a sense or spirit of group unity collectively held by the membership, i.e., as a group property, rather than some mathematical function of individual scores. I have strong feelings about this issue because I do take the gestault position that "groups are real," i.e., have an existence that is more than some mathematical function of the individuals within it. I believe that such a gestault property explains why we must study a family to learn more about individual pathology or understand an institution's structure to explain individual productivity within it (more on this one later.)
Some behavior that appears idiosyncratic or spontaneous to outsiders may, in fact, be normative for that particular group. Several years ago, when I began an ethnographic study of religious congregations, I was startled the first time someone next to me rose and began speaking in tongues. I soon learned, however, that this individual enacted this behavior during practically every service and at the same part of the service each time, then another congregant would translate for her into English. In another congregation, at one part of the service, an elderly gentleman would rise, toss his cane into the aisle and begin dancing. This happened practically every Sunday, again at the same part of the service. Both behaviors were clearly normative for those congregations and a designated person enacted them in each case at a predictable time point.
People may have face-to-face interaction on a daily basis, yet form an aggregate rather than a group ("the elevator crowd," for example). Online groups often have delayed or "asynchronous" interaction, written and somewhat stilted, depending on server speed, yet may strongly identify with membership. For example, I have been part of the "AAPOR-net" (public opinion research) list_serv for over seven years and have never directly met many members. Yet after literally thousands of emails, I "feel" that I "know" many persons on it and sometimes communicate with them off list on more personal issues.
How many members
does it take to form a group? I have seen some definitions
in which only two members (a "dyad") are required. In other definitions,
at least three people must be group members. Keep in mind that, of necessity,
dyads cannot experience subgroups, coalitions, or other mechanisms only
available to groups of at least three.
|
One modern innovation is "groups" that only communicate via the Internet. These may be members of various chat rooms, listservs associated with professional societies, members of Facebook, Twitter or Instagram, or students who are all enrolled in a Distance Learning course. Communication is sometimes delayed and sometimes practically instantaneous. The development of high speed networks with accompanying software makes synchronized communication much more common.
Experimentally, such "groups" are actually an old story. One common experimental paradigm to assess the group effects is to create a bogus group accessed only through computer. Thus, the experimenters can precisely arrange feedback and other information from "group members" to the naive participant. Many studies show that "membership" in and feedback from these contrived computer groups affect behavior, attitudes, and identity. Thus, we should expect online groups to also influence their members, probably even more so, because interaction is more naturalistic and group members typically have elements in common.
But are online groups really groups in the sense of the defining characteristics listed above?
REVIEW THESE DEFINING CHARACTERISTICS HERE!
For obvious reasons, research on online issues is still feeling its way. And it's not easy when the hardware (e.g., Broadband versus dial-up--huh, really?) and software (templates for online portfolios) seem to change almost weekly. However, here are some factors to mention:
However, more recent studies suggest that interaction via the Internet may, in fact, be eroding face to face ties with close friends and relatives.
The work of John Robinson (retired from University of Maryland) and his colleagues on time use and Internet use is relevant.
And Social Science Computer Review and Bulletin of Science, Knowledge & Technology also study social media, online use and its social determinants and effects. FSU personnel can view it by using the FSU Library's "Distance" or possibly the Library's EZ Proxy, which temporarily assigns an FSU internet service provider address to your off-campus computer.
Is there such a thing as "internet addiction"? Current research suggests no more so than, say, television addiction. However, the Internet does provide users who have other addictions (gaming, gambling, pornography, etc) with a 24/7 venue in which to exercise these (the American Psychiatric Association very recently deemed such online activities psychological disorders).
Dare I say: stay tuned?
OVERVIEW |
|
This page was built with
Netscape Composer.
Susan Carol Losh December
31 2017
HAPPY
NEW YEAR 2018!