In these guides, watch for words and phrases in bold, CAPITAL LETTERS, colors, or italics. These indicate words and phrases you should know.
 
GUIDE 1: ORIENTATION
GUIDE 2: INFANCY
GUIDE 3: EARLY CHILDHOOD
GUIDE 4: MIDDLE CHILDHOOD
GUIDE 5: ADOLESCENCE
GUIDE 6: EARLY ADULTHOOD
GUIDE 7: REVISITING CONTROVERSIES: MIDLIFE
GUIDE 8: LATE ADULTHOOD

 
OVERVIEW
This space often will have assignment timelines and information.
SYLLABUS

DEP 5068-01           SPRING 2014

LIFESPAN DEVELOPMENT
SUSAN CAROL LOSH

 
GUIDE TO THE MATERIAL: ONE
INTRODUCTION AND ORIENTATION

 
CONTROVERSIES
CROSS-CUTTING TIME
DEVELOPMENT ACROSS GROUPS
MAJOR THEMES
MAJOR METHODS

ON LINEARITY AND CONTROVERSY

Perhaps the most intuitive way to organize Lifespan Development materials is by age, e.g., infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, etc. When the intuitive way of organizing the material coincides with how novice learners view the material, it's easier to learn.

And there is some sense in organizing development by chronological age. For example:

 
An old joke begins: if you begin toilet training your infant at 6 months, at about what age will  the child be "housebroken?" Answer: two years. If you start toilet training your child at age 20 months, when will the child generally be toilet trained? Answer: two years
So why am I somewhat uneasy about taking such an intuitive, useful, and logical way to organize material on lifespan development as the linear by chronological age orientation?
CONTROVERSIES THAT CUT ACROSS TIME


Nature and nurture issues continue throughout the entire lifespan (does watching too much talk show TV hasten the onset of Alzheimer's? is it cooking in aluminum pans? or not ingesting enough vitamin E?) and crosscut issues of chronological age.
 

Now, some of this interest in jury duty among senior adults undeniably is going to be life-cycle rather than age--and so Rob and I found in our analysis. Older individuals are more often retired and less likely to have fixed schedules; thus they are able to be  more positive about jury duty. Students are the most likely to simply fail to appear, perhaps because of their time conflicts with exams and assignments. Self employed individuals in service jobs (such as lawn service) who lose money when they serve on a jury (state employees don't have their pay docked) also score high as Failures To Appear. When mothers of young children can't postpone or get excused, they are the most resentful about jury service. In other words, it's the constraints (or relaxation of constraints) associated with certain portions of the life cycle that can be important, not typically chronological age per se.

People have the opportunity to have all sorts of experiences as we age: we marry, divorce, become widowed, have children or not, take courses, volunteer, work in paid jobs, get rich (or richer, anyway), go broke, and many other possible experiences. Whenever possible we need to specify what those experiences are or what we think it is that's associated with age that provides the experiences. And, once again, these can cross-cut chronological age groups. An "older" parent myself, I quickly spotted gray hairs among other pre-school parents in the "elderly parents club." Some of my experiences might be comparable to those who became parents in their 20s and other experiences were conditioned by my "elderly status."
 

1. Diana Baumrind and Elizabeth Owens found that mild physical discipline with small children such as spanking, does not seem to produce long-lasting harmful effects. However, they also noted that in the 4 out of the 100 families that they studied intensively where no physical punishment occurred at all, the children were unusually well-behaved, But what's the cause? Perhaps the children behaved so well in these families that the parents were never even tempted to spank their kids, that is, the children's behavior was a cause, and not the effect, of the parents' spanking practices. This would argue for a "nature" perspective, cheerful and cooperative children may experience less corporal punishment. Alternatively, perhaps these were unusually "scary" parents who had terrified their offspring into good behavior without physical punishment--definitely a "nurture" explanation.

2. Several researchers have noted that among men over age 30, married (and cohabiting) men on the average enjoy better mental health than single men over 30. But that brings us back to nature-nurture again. Does marriage (or cohabitation) provide a "protective buffer" effect for men or are men in poor mental health avoided in the romance market?*(see the bottom of this page for one possibility)

A DANGER IN "SINGLE-SHOT" SURVEY DATA: 
DEVELOPMENT OR AGING VERSUS COHORT EFFECTS
When we examine the results on some factor, say, knowledge about basic science or use of information technology, by age, and we gather the data at one point in time, it is highly tempting to interpret any differences we find by chronological age group to the developmental effects of aging. For example, if younger adults score higher on tests of basic science factual knowledge than older adults do, we may explain differences through memory decay--younger people have more recent schooling than older ones and their science knowledge is "fresher". Memories among older adults may have "decayed." It's very popular in the studies you read to interpret the predictor "years of age" as something vague to do with the lifespan process.
As long as we study data from only one point in time, our interpretations have an inherent ambiguity to them. What we think might be aging effects (e.g., memory decay) could instead be the experiences of a particular birth cohort or "generation." Remember when (in the 1960s or 1970s) young people were supposedly "liberals" but "turned conservative" as they grew older--i.e. a maturation or development explanation? Then along came "Generation Xs" who were more politically conservative at younger ages than their "Baby Boomer" older siblings. As a result, in national surveys of adults during the 1980s and early 1990s, it "looked as if" people became more politically liberal as they "aged" into middle age. Apparently period events in the 1960s (President Kennedy and his assassination, the Civil Rights and the Feminist social movements, or the Viet Nam war) caused Baby Boomers who experienced them as youth and young adults to espouse more politically liberal beliefs than those born in earlier or later eras.

Is there really a mid-life crisis? Everyone can point to anecdotal evidence (usually involving men, successive wives and little red sports cars) but how about systematic results? By arranging data by both age and birth cohort, I found that repeated cross-sectional American representative samples of Baby Boomers have whined throughout our entire adult lives while Depression Babies as adults have been more optimistic about life than earlier or later birth cohorts. It only LOOKED like a midlife crisis if you examined data at one time point in the early 1990s when Baby Boomers hit their mid-40s. Look at them 10 years earlier and Baby Boomers whined in their 30s too.

Still not convinced? We keep hearing how the curriculum in the schools has been "dumbed down", how each successive group of students is worse than the ones before or how young people "just naturally" take to computers or the Internet more than Seniors. Well, maybe.. but I know that's not true for information technology--IT. I examined aging versus generational effects from 1983 to 2006 using the National Science Foundation Surveys of Public Understanding of Science and Technology. In fact, controlling generation, older people are more likely to own a home computer and spend more time online (more money to afford it!) Take a look at two of these analyses HERE and HERE TOO (these are only optional to skim if you need data to be convinced).

About 30 years ago, American educators began new methods of science education. Allowing time for "trickle through" out of research universities, one would expect effects of this new training to begin showing up as increased knowledge in Generation X and especially among "Millenials". And so it does. Successive generations of Americans know more science facts, and are especially more knowledgeable about science inquiry. That's even controlling chronological age--which had no effect at all. At the same time, as Americans age, we become more skeptical about pseudoscience claims such as astrology or space alien visitation. This does appear to be an aging (probably due to experience) effect. (OPTIONAL: You can read about it in more depth in a paper I presented at the Royal Society of London in late 2007 HERE.)

WHAT'S THE MORAL? Be VERY careful about drawing conclusions about age and development from data collected at one time point . As practitioners you might hear that troublesome youth are that way because "of their age". Or that a particular generation of career-seekers is just "too fussy." Find studies with as long a time line as you can to fully understand what's happening. Contrast age versus generation effects when you can. In at least some cases, the U.S. government's collection of "social indicators," repeated measures over time and the increasing availability of online databases (OPTIONAL: see the General Social Survey at: http://www.norc.org/GSS+Website/ for one example--it covers 1972 through 2010) can help disentangle age versus cohort effects.

HOW IS DEVELOPMENT INFLUENCED BY GROUP OR CULTURAL EFFECTS?
(and did you take these effects into account?)
EXAMPLE: My students and I collected drawings of teachers, veterinarian, and "scientists" from first, third and fifth graders. The gender of the depicted figures could not be determined for 31% of the teachers the first grade boys drew, 28% of the scientists and 47% of the veterinarians these young boys drew. Comparable failures to determine the gender of the depicted professional among first grade girls were much lower: 3% for teachers, 5% for scientists and 5% of veterinarians. The clarity of the depicted gender in the drawings rose dramatically by third grade (and fifth too), and the sex difference narrowed significantly.

Why? Fine motor control develops somewhat earlier in girls than boys. So does speech. On the other hand, high school boys are more aware of gender discrepancies in pay and sex discrimination in the workplace than girls. (OPTIONAL:If you like, you can read about the draw-a-scientist study HERE)

Other research finds that children in some cultures physically mature somewhat earlier (e.g., U.S.) than in other cultures (e.g., Asian).

Social development may also differ across groups in non-linear patterns. For example, women are more comfortable being assertive at older ages than men; on the other hand, women are more comfortable expressing joy and sorrow at younger ages than men. Again, these are hardly similar linear progressions for both sexes.
 
 
 
So, the questions then become:  whose development? whose lifestage? and how do the patterns as well as the rates differ across differ groups?

There are some of my reasons for the discomfort presenting this material in age-graded order. So, why did I return to that presentational order? To be consistent with your readings and virtually all the texts in the lifespan development field, and, once again, because to present material in a manner consistent with one's intuitive beginning grasp is to scaffold and make the material easier to learn. But don't be surprised if I interject these six major points above throughout the semester for different stages of the lifespan.

MAJOR THEMES IN LIFESPAN DEVELOPMENT

 
Theme One: Developmental Theories and Symbolic Interactionism: Personality versus the Self

The main theoretical perspectives you will see this semester address development stage theories, various learning theories, or a combination of the two. In addition, I will present symbolic interactionist approaches pertaining to the development of the self and evolutionary-anticipatory perspectives.

PERSONALITY APPROACHES
 

The nexus of developmental theories juxtaposed with symbolic interactionist approaches actually create a seventh controversy: that of the concept of "personality" versus the "self". The concept of "personality" appears to consist of sets of personal characteristics, traits, or predispositions to respond, such as being honest, confident, adventurous, ambitious, sensitive, and so on. The concept "personality" also conveys CONSISTENCY AND STABILITY, in our predispositions to respond across time and situations. Thus, we expect "honest people" to be honest across many tempting situations, "confident people" to have high self-efficacy across many situations that might make others insecure, adventurous people to take risks on a regular basis, and "funny people," well, to see the humor in almost any situation. Most modern social and behavioral scientists, including developmentalists,  think of personality characteristics such as humor as  largely "nurture," shaped by your envirnoment and personal histories.


Most developmental theorists no longer designate a fixed point at which "personality" stops developing. Erikson, for example, created theoretical stages that lasted from infancy to old age. In common with other developmentalists, however (e.g., Freud, Piaget, Kohlberg), Erikson crams most of his developmental stages into the early part of one's life, with Stage V generally occurring in the mid-teens. There are several reasons why developmentalists see most "personality creation" occurring in the early years and thus we will focus the most on the early years in this course, both in the text and in class:

Developmentalists are sometimes called "stage theorists" because many of them believe that personality develops in irregular (monotonic) stages, typically keyed to physical development. Experiences and coping with experiences at earlier ages can shape how the individual perceives and responds to events at later stages.


Now this may come as a shock: as defined above, which of course is a very common way for psychologists to use the term, there is remarkably little empirical evidence that you "have a personality," or a bundle of consistent personal traits. And this knowledge is hardly new. In the late 1920s, psychologists Hartshorn and May studied children's honesty across different tempting situations. Instead of consistent tendencies to respond in a generally honest or dishonest manner, Hartshorn and May discovered instead that the situation was the primary determinant of "honest" behavior, and children were strikingly inconsistent instead.

How can this be? And what about all those personality tests circulating out there?

THE SELF

So why do we feel we have a "personality"? For all the reasons below:

We are more likely to view the behavior of others as being driven by their personalities than our own behavior, which we tend to view as situationally determined by the environment.  If we see ourselves on video or listen to ourselves on audio tape, we have a stronger impression that we were the causes of our own behavior, rather than situational events.

To symbolic interactionists, the self is a concept, an image or schema of our persona, and it is both subject and object.  That is, the subjective self acts, "the I" initiates action, it dreams, plans and thinks (akin to Freud's notion of "ego." ) At the same time, the self is reflexive or an object: a "me;" we think about our self, plan for our self, and view the self as we do another person. The self is an on-going social construction; it is never finished. We don't think of the self as "being complete" at any particular age.

It is because of the self, or our perception of a unified entity, that we believe that we "have a personality." But, although we view our self as having consistent tendencies to act, the self is not the same kind of construct as a set of personality traits.

We will look more at determinants of one's "self" as we go through the semester.

 The "self" is not simply another word for "personality."The construct of personality is considered to be:

Once "established," personality is considered relatively permanent.

The self, or more accurately, the collection of selves many symbolic interactionists believe comprise most of us, is:

IMPLICATIONS FOR CLINICAL PRACTICE:

The idea of "the self" as opposed to "personality" actually opens opportunities and magnifies the importance of the psychological practitioner. This is because the constantly evolving self is more open in theory to clinical practice and suggestions and able to change. It means you shouldn't assume that an adult's career goals are "fixed" or that a psychological problem is necessarily long-standing.
 


 
Theme two: Nature versus Nurture 

Prior to the nineteenth century and even early into the twentieth century, the behavioral and social sciences were dominated by "biology is destiny" theories of personality. Ancient Greek philosophers discussed "bodily humours." Western and Asian astrologers cited the influence of time of the year--or even the very year--of one's birth as bestowing personality traits. Freud's theories of unconscious conflicts among ego, id and superego, the physical, the rational, and societal demands, spread among scholars worldwide.

By the start of the twentieth century, three important streams of thought challenged each other. (1) Social Darwinism, was a somewhat misstated version of Darwin's theories of evolution. The "most fit" were seen as most likely to survive and dominate society. Each human behavior was viewed in terms of how it contributed to the "fittest" of the "human race." These streams of thought led some to conclusions that we now find ludicrous. For example, the disease cholera, an infection spread through poor sanitation (and re-emerging in countries torn by war or natural disaster), was recharacterized as "proof" that the lower classes were less fit. Educated women were depicted as thin and pale, weakened by their studies, and contrasted with supposedly sturdier,more fertile peasant girls. Anticipatory theories also tend to have a biological base and relate to Social Darwinism. For example, it has been pointed out that young boys and girls behave similarly in many ways, so why do they receive such different toys and are often urged to play different games (e.g., sports versus "house")? Such theorists, who often include anthropologists, argue that we are socializing girls and boys to anticipate their adult roles and that children will gravitate on their own to sex-typed toys and games, even if adults do not provide them.

(2) A second important paradigm was emerging that came to dominate twentieth century thought on personality: Pavlov in Russia and Watson in the U.S. felt that in the stimulus-response (S-R) linkage, they had discovered the equivalent of the cell in biology or the atom in physics. From the stimulus-response or behaviorist perspective, the person was a tabula rosa or blank slate. S-R connections were all. Radical in its perspective, behaviorists proposed that occupational destination was a matter of conditioning; the social class differences that exist occurred because of childhood and adult socialization. Cultural differences, often previously defined as biologically based, were redefined as a matter of learning. It was not too much of a hop or a skip to define any sex differences as determined by learning experiences and reinforcement contingencies. We will examine different learning theories later this semester.

(3) The third, or developmental paradigm, partially described above, grew out of Freud's work, as well as research by Jean Piaget in Switzerland, Lev Vygotsky in Russia, and Erickson in the United States.  Biology grounds the individual in particular stages of development, determining what is possible at specific ages.

We are learning that biological development proceeds for longer time periods than once was thought. For example, research over the past several years has discovered that parts of the brain involved with delay of gratification or judgement do not mature until the early 20s--just at a time that many young adults are making decisions about work and family (OPTIONAL: view a synopsis at the National Institute of Mental Health HERE). Furthermore, experiences can reshape brain function (one important reason why working with autistic children should begin as early as possible.)

Developmentalists accept the influence of cultural factors and personal socialization histories. Most propose that stage of development interacts with environment to not only produce behavior, but wishes, motivations, and internal conflicts.

By the mid-1970s, the dominant paradigm was clearly behaviorist, i.e., learning theories. Behavior (perhaps with a bit of developmental modification) was seen as totally shaped by environmental circumstances. Any hint of biological differences was viewed as racist, sexist--and inaccurate.

Then what happened?

DNA happened. Gigantic steps forward in genetic research happened. Pharmaceutical advances in the treatment of schizophrenia, depression and mania happened. We could actually begin to locate genes for alcoholism. Emotional dysfunction, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder or agoraphobia, or bipolar disorder, came to some extent under pharmaceutical influence. Prozac and Zoloft became household words. All these advances have cast doubt on "personality" being solely determined by environmental and stimulus-response causes.

    THEME THREE: CLASSIC "IDEAL-TYPE" DEVELOPMENTAL VERSUS LEARNING THEORIES: AN OVERVIEW

CONCEPT
DEVELOPMENTAL
LEARNING THEORIES
How development proceeds Fixed sequential stages; none can be skipped. Earlier stages should be completed before later ones. No fixed stages. 
Behavior results from exposure (imitation) and reinforcement contingencies. Learned from others, but in no particular order.
Completing life tasks and challenges successfully at each stage Skipping stages or having conflicts at one stage create problems at later stages, even during adulthood. Problems that seem linked to a particular stage are just habits that were once rewarded and are high on the habit hierarchy.
What determines stages? Biology, somewhat, and organism's capacity for physical bodily control which can be linked to age. Maturational factors such as physical control or linguistic development can influence what you are capable of learning at a particular age.
Importance of environment Environment can cause problems with "normal development;" can speed or slow development. Environment essentially all-important for what is learned.


MAJOR METHODS IN LIFESPAN DEVELOPMENT

Like other behavioral and social sciences, studies in lifespan development use experiments (somewhat less than other fields), surveys and observation (both somewhat more than other fields). Many of the variables that developmentalists are interested in, such as chronological age, do not lend themselves to experimentation. On the other hand, interest in the development of social interaction, speech, and reasoning mean that developmentalists are often more likely to use structured observation, ethnographies of the life course, or in-depth interviews more than other branches of science.

(OPTIONAL:Want a basic refresher on these basic methods? Click HERE)

There are also methods that lifespan development scholars are much more likely to use that come with their own set of warnings.

For example, lifespan researchers have an obvious interest in archival records which can enable them to track development. What happened to the "at-risk" students in elementary school? How were their grades in high school and what is their rate of high school graduation compared with other students? How many families that go on welfare remain on welfare throughout their working lives? Who is more likely to remarry and for how long: divorce(e)s or widow(er)s?

The biggest caution here is that we often don't know the human factors influencing how the archival records are created. To "spare the family," physicians are reluctant to put "suicide" as a cause of death on official records, thus providing part of the explanation of why unmarried persons are more likely to kill themselves than wives or husbands. Records are often incomplete or go missing. If there are any systematic influences (e.g., better educated persons create more complete records) on the records, these may create a bias.

Lifespan development scholars also are more likely to use longitudinal panel studies. The "longitudinal" part means tracking over time. The "panel" part means that the same people are repeatedly studied. Longitudinal panel data help us understand "net change" over time. We can see what actually happened to the kid with the poor elementary school grades or to the child diagnosed with "attention deficit-hyperactive disorder". We can see how much attitudes remain the same or change as someone undergoes life cycle changes.

However the advantage of longitudinal panel studies carries a major disadvantage: reactivity, which refers to changes that are a result of the study treatment. Being repeatedly observed or interviewed can change participant behavior in ways that have nothing to do with the entities under study, but instead with study procedures themselves. At the least, repeated scrutiny can create or increase self-monitoring among participants. It can also literally change behaviors.

One way to check for possible biases due to repeated scrutiny is to compare longitudinal panel results with repeated cross-sectional studies. In repeated cross-sectional studies, variables of interest are studied over time, but a "fresh" sample from the same population is obtained each time. The General Social Survey noted above is one example using a general population sample of U.S. adults; it now also has a panel study that can be used for comparison purposes. The new sample participants in repeated cross-sectional studies each time are "naive" to the study purposes and should be comparable to individuals of the same age and birth cohort studied during the same period from longitudinal panel studies.

However, many longitudinal panel studies (the Minnesota twins study, the Oakland California study) use relatively specialized populations and samples from those populations. It may be prohibitively expensive to locate and sample comparable popuations using repeated cross-sectional samples. And, any sampling biases in either the panel study or the repeated cross-sectional samples must be noted and described.

Repeated cross-sectional studies only allow us to examine "gross change" in the population as a whole, not the "net change" that occurs among individuals. There may be selective attrition as we track birth cohorts or generations over time that may introduce bias. For example, better educated individuals are more likely to have healthy living habits that enable them to live longer and higher income individuals have better access to good medical care. Selective attrition can occur both in longitudinal panel studies and in repeated cross-sectional designs, making controls for variables involved in such attrition critical.

Lifespan developmentalists are more likely than scholars in many other fields to use cross-cultural research. Results from other countries and from other times are expected to help shed light on what is more "nature" and what is more "nurture". For example, cross-cultural studies indicate that older daughters are more likely to be set in charge of younger siblings than older sons. It is very tough these days to find "untouched cultures"--TV, cell phones and the Internet are virtually everywhere. Thus it is also harder to tease out what is "unique" about a culture. The Human Relations Area Files are one classic source of data about many different cultures.

OPTIONAL:Click HERE for more information about the Human Relations Area Files.

I hope it's clear by this time that this is no ONE method to study lifespan development. The hope is that we will use several methods that will converge or "triangulate" in their findings, thereby adding to the confidence in our findings.
 
 
OVERVIEW

This page was built with Netscape Composer.
Susan Carol Losh January 6 2014


*Men, marriage/cohabitation and mental health: it took several years of longitudinal panel surveys to be able to disentangle nature and nurture on this one. Notice how one can make a plausible argument for either causal direction. Analyses of the University of Michigan's longitudinal study of high school seniors finds that married or cohabiting men are less likely to go to bars, less likely to drink excessively, and enjoy better mental health than non-married/cohabiting men. When the marriage or relationship breaks up, men are more likely to go to bars and/or drink in the aftermath and their mental health declines.